Skip to main content

Increasing myopic refraction reduces random dot stereopsis in Chinese myopic patients: a cross-sectional study

Abstract

Objective

To investigate the effects of myopic spherical equivalent (SE) on random dot stereopsis and influencing factors in Chinese adults with myopia.

Methods

A cross-sectional design was employed, and 988 Chinese myopic individuals (520 [52.6%] females) aged 18.0–48.7 years were recruited from the People’s Hospital of Guangxi. The participants underwent assessments for visual acuity, myopic SE, random dot stereopsis at 0.8 m (RDS0.8) and at 1.5 m (RDS1.5) and binocular function parameters (such as perceptual eye position (PEP), fixational eye movement, and the signal‒noise ratio (SNR)). The data were analysed via Pearson or Spearman correlations and multivariate logistic regression.

Results

Among the 988 participants, only 53 (5.4%) presented with abnormal RDS0.8, and 834 (84.4%) presented with abnormal RDS1.5. A significant association was found between SE and the prevalence of abnormal RDS1.5 (OR: 1.14, 95% CI: 1.03–1.26; P = 0.014) after adjusting for covariates. High myopia was more strongly associated with abnormal RDS1.5 than mild myopia was in the unadjusted model (OR: 1.85, 95% CI: 1.03–3.26; P = 0.037). Subgroup analyses revealed that the associations between SE and abnormal RDS1.5 were stronger among females, individuals aged > 25 years, those with normal fixational eye movement, and those with abnormal SNRs. Only vertical PEP (target 1°) was significantly associated with myopic group and abnormal RDS0.8, whereas vertical PEP (target 3°), horizontal PEP (target 1°), and vertical PEP (target 1°) were significantly associated with abnormal RDS1.5.

Conclusions

Myopic SE was associated with the prevalence of abnormal random dot stereopsis at 1.5 m in myopic patients, indicating that increasing myopic SE may impair distance random dot stereopsis in this population.

Clinical trial number

Not applicable.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Myopia is a significant global health concern that is particularly prevalent in East Asia [1]. This refractive error not only impairs distant vision but also affects complex visual functions such as stereopsis [2]. Stereopsis, a fundamental process for 3D perception, involves intricate neural mechanisms from generating binocular disparity to constructing depth perception [3]. Impairments in stereopsis significantly impact daily activities, such as distance estimation and hand‒eye coordination [4].

Although the effects of myopia on basic visual functions such as acuity are well documented, its impact on random dot stereopsis is less understood. A previous study revealed that, compared with emmetropes, myopes presented decreased stereopsis [5]. Among adults, stereopsis was significantly poorer in the high myopia group than in the moderate-to-low myopia group [6, 7]. In contrast, one study suggested that the severity of myopia does not affect stereoacuity in school-aged children [8]. However, some studies have shown that myopic eyes exhibit reduced efficiency in processing blurred information under monocular conditions. This deficiency improves under binocular conditions through binocular central summation [9, 10]. Additionally, under specific conditions, such as low spatial frequencies and high motion speeds, individuals with higher degrees of myopia tend to have lower motion detection thresholds in certain visual field regions, such as the nasal and superior fields. These findings indicate that myopia may partially affect peripheral motion perception [11, 12]. The ability to utilize stereoscopic depth information relies on the efficiency of processing binocular retinal images; a decline in this efficiency can affect stereopsis thresholds. A previous study revealed that myopic patients experience decreased stereoscopic vision function at specific spatial and temporal frequencies because of the weakened processing capabilities of binocular images at these frequencies, making it more difficult to judge depth accurately [13]. Collectively, these studies suggest a potential link between myopia and impaired stereopsis. However, owing to limitations such as small sample sizes and heterogeneous assessment methods, the relationship between myopia and stereopsis remains unclear.

Therefore, the present study was designed to examine the effects of the spherical equivalent on random dot stereopsis in Chinese adults with myopia. Furthermore, the effects of visual function parameters (such as division, fusion, perceptual eye position, fixational eye movements, and binocular balance points) on this relationship were analysed. Employing a robust cross-sectional methodology, we wanted to understand whether there was an association between myopia and random dot stereopsis, offering insights that may guide future therapeutic and clinical strategies to preserve and enhance binocular vision in myopic individuals.

Materials and methods

Study design and study participants

This cross-sectional study enrolled 988 Chinese adults with myopia aged between 18.0 and 48.7 years at the Optometry Clinic of People’s Hospital of Guangxi from March 1 to September 30, 2022. The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) a myopic spherical equivalent (SE) of at least 0.50 diopters (D), with astigmatism less than 2.00 D and anisometropia less than 2.00 D [14,15,16]; and (2) participants aged 18 years or older with a best-corrected visual acuity (BCVA) of 0.1 LogMAR or better (equivalent to 20/25 or better). The exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) inability to cooperate with the examination process; (2) a history of eye surgery; (3) the presence of binocular alignment issues or motor dysfunction, such as strabismus or nystagmus; (4) ocular diseases other than myopia; and (5) a history of systemic diseases such as heart, liver, or kidney disorders or mental health issues.

Measurement

The participants underwent assessments for visual acuity, myopic refraction, and visual function.

Measurement of visual acuity and refraction

Visual acuity was assessed monocularly by a skilled optometrist (XX) via an E-letter standard logarithmic visual acuity chart (SJ-LED-01, Guangzhou Shijia Medical Corporation, Guangzhou, China) at a 5-meter distance. The myopic spherical equivalent (SE) was determined through subjective refraction, and best-corrected visual acuity (BCVA) was recorded via the LogMAR scale. Anisometropia was defined as an interocular spherical equivalent difference of 1.00 D or more [15].

Measurement of binocular visual function

The study employed a battery of tests by ophthalmologists (YL, LL, EL, MK, and QC) to evaluate binocular visual function (Fig. 1), including tasks such as division, fusion, perceptual eye position (PEP) [17, 18], fixational eye movement, binocular balance point assessment by the signal‒noise ratio (SNR) [19], and stereopsis measured via random dot stereograms at near (0.8 m from the screen, RDS0.8) and distance (1.5 m from the screen, RDS1.5) [17, 20], developed by the National Engineering Research Center for Healthcare Devices. The stimuli were generated via MATLAB and displayed on a high-resolution polarized monitor (LGD2343P, 1980 × 1080 pixels and 120 Hz refresh rate). All tests were conducted in a consistent lighting room with refractive correction provided by spectacles. All participants underwent the tests by a skilled operator, and each assessment was repeated at least three times to ensure accuracy by calculating the average data. All examinations were performed with best-corrected visual acuity for all participants.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart of this study. PEP: perceptual eye position; SNR: signal‒noise ratio

Measurement of division and fusion

The participants were positioned at a distance of 0.8 m from the screen, with their eyes aligned with the screen’s height, and were instructed to maintain a stable head and body position while viewing the images. The participants looked at the two vertical lines on the screen via 3D polarized glasses. The examiner gradually increased the distance between the two vertical lines until the participants could not merge the two lines into one line (negative values represent division). The two lines were then moved in the opposite direction until the participants could not merge the two lines into one line again (positive values represent fusion).

Measurement of the PEP

The PEP test was used to assess fixational disparity and binocular visual function. It was assessed via the cross-into-circle test, in which a cross was presented to the left eye and a circle was presented to the right eye. The midpoint of the monitor was positioned 80 cm away and at the same level as the participants’ eyes, with an average luminance of 80 cd/m2 in white, decreasing to 50 cd/m2 with the use of 3D polarized glasses, and 30 cd/m2 in black, decreasing to 3 cd/m2 with 3D polarized glasses. The visual stimulus template measured 51 × 29 cm in physical size and subtended a visual angle of 38° × 18°. The circle had dimensions of 0.4 × 0.4°, whereas the cross measured 0.33 × 0.33° (1° fixation test-object). The participants utilized a computer mouse to position the cross at the perceived center of the circle and were subsequently directed to click the mouse. The PEP tests were conducted according to methods reported in previous studies [15, 17, 18].

Measurement of fixational eye movement

Fixational eye movement examinations were conducted with a Tobii Eye Tracker 5 (Tobii Company, Sweden), operating at a sampling rate of 133 Hz. The average binocular accuracy of the eye tracker ranged from 0.5 to 1 degree from the visual angle. This noninvasive device allowed unrestricted head movements during testing. All assessments were efficiently completed within minutes in a controlled testing environment characterized by quiet and uniform illumination. The participants were positioned at a distance of 0.8 m from the screen, with their eyes aligned with the screen’s height, and were instructed to maintain a stable head and body position while viewing the images. Prior to each examination, a three-point calibration was conducted to ensure the accuracy and precision of gaze tracking. A circular target with a diameter of 10 degrees was presented above the foveal view in a series of nine repetitions per trial. Each target appeared for 2 s with a 0.5-second interval. The eye tracker followed the targets within a 1-degree peripheral visual field, moving at a velocity threshold of 1 degree per second. Relative deviation values in the horizontal and vertical directions from the circular target were recorded and averaged [21].

Measurement of the binocular balance point using the SNR

The participant utilized 3D polarized glasses to view stimuli on a screen, consisting of dichoptically presented signal and noise dots within a frame. Initially, the right eye viewed the signal, and the left eye viewed the noise. The signal dots moved uniformly in all directions, and the noise dots moved randomly. This arrangement was subsequently reversed. The participants had to identify the direction of the signal movement. The noise-to-signal dot ratio was adjusted until interference from the opposite eye’s noise made the signal’s direction indiscernible, establishing the binocular balance point. The signal‒noise ratio was adjusted through eight levels, starting with 100% signal dots at level 1 and decreasing by 10% per level down to 20% at level 8, with a corresponding increase in noise dots. This test measures how well the signal eye resists noise interference, reflecting binocular balance ability. Each level involved three trials; passing required correct identification, which allowed progression to the next level. The binocular signal‒noise ratios were recorded as balance point values [22].

Measurement of random dot stereopsis

A random dot distribution stereogram with a luminance of 44 cd/m2 on a gray background of 125 was displayed on a 3D monitor. Within a 5° × 5° square area, 1250 random dots on a gray background of 250 were arranged. The participants observed a central optotype, an E-target measuring 3° × 3°, positioned within the central region of the random dot distribution map. The surrounding random dots served as a reference for relative nonparallax. The participant utilized 3D polarized glasses and was directed to identify the orientation of the protruding E-target aperture displayed on the screen by selecting the corresponding arrow icon via mouse or keyboard input. Various levels of disparity, including 400”, 300”, 200”, and 100”, were utilized. Initially, the participant observed a protruding E-target with a size of 400” and was tasked with identifying the aperture orientation twice. Successful identification on both occasions resulted in the presentation of a smaller protruding E-target measuring 300”, with subsequent reductions in size continuing until reaching 100”. If the participant provided an incorrect response, the test would revert to the previous higher level of discrepancy. The outcome was documented, with assessment distances categorized as 0.8 m (RDS0.8) and 1.5 m (RDS1.5). The principle and detection process of the random dot stereopsis test was described in a previous study [23], and the results showed that the random dot stereopsis test results are consistent with the Titmus test results (Kappa = 0.493).

Definition

Myopia was divided into three groups according to SE: mild myopia (SE >-3.00 D and ≤-0.50 D), moderate myopia (SE > -6.00 D and ≤ -3.00 D) and high myopia (SE ≤ -6.00 D). Various levels of random dot stereopsis at 0.8 m (RDS0.8) and 1.5 m (RDS1.5), including 400”, 300”, 200”, and 100”. Normal stereopsis was defined as 100”, and abnormal stereopsis was defined as > 100” (400”, 300”, and 200”). The SNRs of the right eye or left eye were recorded at levels 1 to 8. A normal SNR is defined as the same SNR level for the left and right eyes. An abnormal SNR was defined as the difference between the SNR level of the right eye and the SNR level of the left eye (e.g., an SNR level of 2 for the right eye and an SNR level of 3 for the left eye). Fixational eye movement results are divided into four levels: stable, normal, shaking, and running. Normal fixational eye movements were defined as stable and normal. Abnormal fixational eye movements were defined as shaking and running.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted via R, a popular software package (version 4.3.1, The R Foundation, released on 2024-05-06; http://www.R-project.org). The distribution of measurement data was assessed through the Shapiro–Wilk test, with the results reported as the means ± SDs for normally distributed data or medians (P25, P75) for nonnormally distributed data. Correlation tests were performed with Pearson’s method for normally distributed data and Spearman’s test for nonnormally distributed data.

To investigate the relationship between the spherical equivalent (SE) and the prevalence of abnormal random dot stereopsis, a multivariate logistic regression model was employed. Model 1 included age and sex as covariates; Model 2 added anisometropia, astigmatism, and BCVA to Model 1 [20]. In addition, the stability of the study results was verified via subgroup analyses. Significance was set at P < 0.05.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants

From March 1 to September 30, 2022, a total of 1102 Chinese adults with myopia were recruited, and 988 patients, aged 18.0 to 48.7 years, completed the test at the Optometry Clinic of People’s Hospital of Guangxi. The study flowchart is depicted in Fig. 1. Table 1 shows significant differences in sex, age, degree of anisometropia, SE, astigmatism, vertical PEP (target 1°), SNR, and RDS1.5 among the mild, moderate, and high myopia groups (all P < 0.05).

Table 1 Characteristics of the participants in the myopia group

Association between myopic SE and abnormal random dot stereopsis

Among the 988 participants, only 53 (5.4%) presented with abnormal RDS0.8, and 834 (84.4%) presented with abnormal RDS1.5 (Table 1). Table 2 displays the associations between these factors and the risk of abnormal RDS0.8, with univariate analysis revealing significant associations with the degree of anisometropia and vertical PEP (target 1°) ( both P < 0.05). However, there were no significant correlations between myopia and RDS0.8 (Table 2).

Table 2 Associations between covariates and abnormal RDS0.8 risk via univariate logistic regression analysis

Participants with abnormal RDS1.5 had greater SEs than those with normal RDS1.5 (P < 0.001; Fig. 2). SE was significantly associated with abnormal RDS1.5 based on correlation analysis (Table 3; Fig. 3). Therefore, multivariate logistic regression analysis and subgroup analyses were performed using RDS1.5 data. However, abnormal RDS1.5 was not significantly correlated with sex, age, degree of anisometropia, division, fusion, fixational eye movement or the SNR (Table 3; Fig. 3). After adjustment for influential confounders in Model 2, the associations between SE and abnormal RDS1.5 remained significant (OR = 1.14, 95% CI: 1.03–1.26, P = 0.014; Table 4). Furthermore, high myopia was more strongly associated with abnormal RDS1.5 than mild myopia was (OR = 1.85, 95% CI: 1.03–3.26, P = 0.037; Table 4) according to the unadjusted model, whereas adjustment for covariates revealed that high myopia was not associated with abnormal RDS1.5 compared with mild myopia (P > 0.05; Table 4). Subgroup analyses revealed that the associations between SE and abnormal RDS1.5 were stronger among females (OR = 1.19, 95% CI: 1.02–1.39, P = 0.027; Fig. 4), individuals aged > 25 years (OR = 1.23, 95% CI: 1.06–1.44, P = 0.008; Fig. 4), those with normal fixational eye movement, and those with abnormal SNRs.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Boxplot and scatter plot of myopic SE against abnormal RDS1.5. The horizontal axis represents stereopsis measured by random dot stereograms at 1.5 m, whereas the vertical axis represents myopic spherical equivalent. SE: spherical equivalent; RDS1.5: stereopsis measured via random dot stereograms at 1.5 m from the screen

Table 3 Associations between covariates and abnormal RDS1.5 risk via univariate logistic regression analysis
Fig. 3
figure 3

Heatmap of correlations between covariates and abnormal RDS1.5. SE: all myopic values were reported as absolute values; RDS1.5: stereopsis measured via random dot stereograms at 1.5 m from the screen; RDS0.8: stereopsis measured via random dot stereograms at 0.8 m from the screen; BCVA: best-corrected visual acuity; LogMAR: logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution; PEP_Hori_Big: horizontal PEP (target 3°); PEP_Vect_Big: vertical PEP (target 3°); PEP_Hori_Small: horizontal PEP (target 1°); PEP_Vect_Small: vertical PEP (target 1°); SNR: signal‒noise ratio

Table 4 Associations between myopic SE and abnormal RDS1.5 risk among myopic patients
Fig. 4
figure 4

Forest plot of subgroup analysis of the association between myopic SE and abnormal RDS1.5 by covariates. SE: all myopic values were reported as absolute values; SNR: signal‒noise ratio; OR: odds ratio; RDS1.5: stereopsis measured via random dot stereograms at 1.5 m from the screen

Associations between visual functions and abnormal random Dot stereopsis

Fixational eye movement was not significantly different among the study groups (Table 1) and was not associated with abnormal RDS0.8 (Table 2; Fig. 3) or abnormal RDS1.5 in the correlation analysis (Table 3; Fig. 3). However, the associations between SE and abnormal RDS1.5 were stronger in individuals with normal fixational eye movement (OR = 1.21; 95% CI: 1.07–1.37, P = 0.002; Fig. 4).

The SNR was significantly different among the study groups (Table 1), but there was no association with abnormal RDS0.8 (Table 2; Fig. 3) or abnormal RDS1.5 in the correlation analysis (Table 3; Fig. 3). However, in the abnormal SNR group, SE was more strongly correlated with abnormal RDS1.5 (OR = 1.28, 95% CI: 1.09–1.52, P = 0.004; Fig. 4).

In terms of perceptual eye position, only the vertical PEP (target 1°) significantly differed among the study groups (Table 1). Only the vertical PEP (target 1°) was significantly associated with an abnormal RDS0.8 in the correlation analysis (P < 0.05; Table 2; Fig. 3). Vertical PEP (target 3°), horizontal PEP (target 1°), and vertical PEP (target 1°) were significantly associated with abnormal RDS1.5 in the correlation analysis (all P < 0.05; Table 3; Fig. 3).

Discussions

In this study, we investigated how spherical equivalent relates to random dot stereopsis abnormalities at different distances in Chinese myopic patients. We found a greater prevalence of random dot stereopsis abnormalities in patients with more severe myopia, especially those with high myopia. These findings suggest that myopia may affect random dot stereopsis, resulting in differences in visual function impairment based on myopia severity. Individuals with anisometropia often have stereopsis problems [17]. The prevalence of abnormal stereopsis in the general population varies from 3 to 70% [5, 15, 17, 24, 25]. This report of the prevalence of abnormal RDS0.8 (5.36%) was consistent with previous studies among the myopic population [5]. However, the rate of anomalies was much higher in RDS1.5 (84.4%). Near and distance stereopsis represent different aspects of binocular vision; near vision enhances depth perception through increased binocular convergence, whereas distant vision, with parallel eyes, often results in reduced stereopsis [26]. Previous studies have focused on near stereopsis and overlooked the importance of distance stereopsis [17, 27,28,29]. Patients with high myopia exhibited significant abnormalities in random dot stereopsis at 1.5 m, followed by moderate and mild myopia in this study. Patients with myopia may have difficulty with relative static stereopsis due to imbalanced binocular competition and mild interocular suppression [30]. This may be related to changes in eye structure, such as elongation of the axial length, which can stretch the retina, impair photoreceptor function, and affect stereopsis [31].

Compared with emmetropes, individuals with myopia presented weaker stereopsis and greater binocular imbalance at higher spatial and lower temporal frequencies [13]. This finding indicates that depth perception in myopia is influenced by alterations in binocular image processing, making it more difficult to judge depth accurately under certain viewing conditions. Our results are in agreement with the results of that analysis. One possible explanation for this phenomenon is abnormalities in magnocellular pathways. The signals of the magnocellular pathway are located primarily within the peripheral retina, where they play a significant role in depth perception and motion detection [32, 33]. Therefore, structural and functional alterations associated with ocular elongation in myopia may impair magnocellular processing, resulting in decreased stereopsis.

Previous studies have examined the link between myopia and stereopsis but have not considered the influence of sex and age. This study investigated the relationship between myopia and random dot stereopsis abnormalities, with a focus on sex and age. A significant positive correlation between myopia and stereopsis abnormalities was detected in females compared with males. Significant differences in visual acuity and refractive status were observed in female myopic patients during different menstrual cycles, possibly due to the role of estrogen in regulating eye components [34]. Changes in female hormone levels can also cause temporary visual blurriness by affecting tear film stability [34]. Moreover, women are more likely to develop high myopia than men are, possibly because men spend more time outdoors, which can slow the progression of myopia [35, 36]. This study also revealed more women with moderate to high myopia, suggesting that they may be more susceptible to stereopsis abnormalities. Moreover, we discovered that age also plays a significant role in the relationship between myopia and stereopsis abnormalities. A previous study revealed that defective stereopsis is common in the elderly population (aged > 65 years). These findings indicate that an age-related decrease in ocular structure and cerebral function may worsen the impact on stereopsis [37]. However, the relatively young age of the participants in this study (25.4 ± 6.18 years) was attributable to the clinical setting, as most subjects were enrolled from a refractive surgery outpatient clinic, where many patients were young adults seeking myopia correction. Thus, further studies should expand the scope of the study population to confirm this viewpoint.

In vision science, noise can interfere with the process of transforming light into electrical signals in the retina, ultimately affecting visual perception [38, 39]. Compared with background noise, the strength of neural responses, known as the SNR, plays a significant role in this association. The perceptual template model (PTM) proposes that the brain utilizes both internal and external mechanisms to reduce noise, but too much noise can hinder this ability [40]. The SNR can be used to evaluate binocular imbalance [41]. This study revealed that the greater the severity of myopia is, the worse the stereopsis is in binocular imbalance individuals. Short-term binocular imbalance is typically a temporary physiological occurrence that does not harm visual acuity or function [42]. It should be considered pathological only if it persists and leads to amblyopia [43]. The brain selects a clearer image that may damage stereopsis during long-term binocular imbalance [44].

Previous doubts about fixational eye movements as a hindrance to visual perception have been made [45]. That is, the stronger the fixational eye movements are, the worse the visual function is [46]. However, in light of our findings, this view warrants reconsideration. We found that stable and normal fixational eye movements were linked to a stronger connection between myopia and stereopsis abnormalities. This result suggested that people need to constantly adjust eye movements (micro saccades and ocular drifts) to precisely position the retinal image and achieve stereoscopic images rather than complete stillness in both eyes [47]. Generally, eye movements are common during periods of fixation in real-life situations. Our previous study reported that healthy individuals make quick eye movements between target positions to maintain perception and spatial orientation [21]. These fixational eye movements are crucial for normal visual tasks, especially dynamic mobility vision tasks. Stereopsis involves being able to see and differentiate small differences in depth, which helps people notice subtle 3D details. Problems with this could greatly affect daily activities, such as driving, sports, hand‒eye coordination tasks, and art appreciation [14, 48,49,50]. This study revealed a link between severe myopia and difficulties with stereopsis at a distance of 1.5 m. Consequently, it is imperative to conduct comprehensive visual function assessments for myopic patients, especially those with high myopia. The timely identification of random dot stereopsis impairments and the implementation of appropriate interventions, in addition to correcting refractive errors, are crucial. Vision function training, which capitalizes on neural plasticity, involves a series of visual training tasks to increase the ability of the brain to process binocular disparities and stereopsis in various environments. This training could facilitate improved adaptation to visual challenges in daily activities, ultimately enhancing overall quality of life.

Abnormal eye position and stereopsis are the main clinical manifestations of strabismus [17] and amblyopia [51]. We found the same relationship between PEP and stereopsis among myopic populations. Sensory fusion can be maintained through precise adjustments in eye positions to align the binocular images. Only then is normal stereopsis possible to achieve [52, 53]. The traditional eye position assessed by the Hirschberg and cover tests indicates apparent eye alignment. However, the PEP reflects the brain’s integration of visual input via a computer-controlled perceptual examination system under dichoptic conditions. This test is more precise than the synoptophore test. Therefore, individuals with a normal apparent eye position may present PEP abnormalities [18].

This study revealed a link between SE and abnormal stereopsis in myopic patients, but the design limits conclusions about causality. In addition, the age range of the participants included in the present study was rather small. Children and the elderly population were not included in the sample. Prospective studies with larger samples are needed to further validate the association between myopia and stereopsis. This is the next step of our ongoing study, expanding the study to include children and adolescents to assess refractive status and visual function changes, as well as their impact on individual behavior. This approach can clarify the causal relationships among refractive error, stereopsis, and behavioral outcomes. Further study is also recommended to evaluate whether visual function training can enhance stereopsis in myopic individuals, particularly those with abnormal visual function.

In conclusion, this study revealed that SE affects distance random dot stereopsis in Chinese myopic patients, especially in females, those aged over 25 years, and patients with normal fixational eye movements and abnormal SNRs. These results highlight the need for tailored management strategies for myopia and offer insights for targeted clinical interventions.

Data availability

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

References

  1. Holden BA, Fricke TR, Wilson DA, Jong M, Naidoo KS, Sankaridurg P, Wong TY, Naduvilath TJ, Resnikoff S. Global prevalence of myopia and high myopia and Temporal trends from 2000 through 2050. Ophthalmology. 2016;123(5):1036–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Congdon N, Wang Y, Song Y, Choi K, Zhang M, Zhou Z, Xie Z, Li L, Liu X, Sharma A, et al. Visual disability, visual function, and myopia among rural Chinese secondary school children: the Xichang pediatric refractive error study (X-PRES)—Report 1. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008;49(7):2888–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Blake R, Wilson HR. Neural models of stereoscopic vision. Trends Neurosci. 1991;14(10):445–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Ooi TL, He ZJ. Space perception of strabismic observers in the real world environment. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015;56(3):1761–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Liu F, Zhao J, Han T, Shen Y, Li M, Liu J, Yang D, Fang Y, Yan L, Zhou X. Screening for stereopsis using an Eye-Tracking Glasses-Free display in adults: A pilot study. Front Med. 2021;8:814908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Elamurugan V, Shankaralingappa P, Aarthy G, Kasturi N, Babu RK. Assessment of stereopsis in pediatric and adolescent spectacle-corrected refractive error - A cross-sectional study. Indian J Ophthalmol. 2022;70(2):604–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Jabbarvand M, Hashemian H, Khodaparast M, Anvari P. Changes in stereopsis after photorefractive keratectomy. J Cataract Refract Surg. 2016;42(6):899–903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Yang JW, Huang TY, Lee JS, Yeung L, Lin YF, Sun CC. Correlation between myopic ametropia and stereoacuity in school-aged children in Taiwan. Jpn J Ophthalmol. 2013;57(3):316–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Maiello G, Walker L, Bex PJ, Vera-Diaz FA. Blur perception throughout the visual field in myopia and emmetropia. J Vis. 2017;17(5):3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Ghosh A, Zheleznyak L, Barbot A, Jung H, Yoon G. Neural adaptation to peripheral blur in myopes and emmetropes. Vision Res. 2017;132:69–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Wei J, Kong D, Yu X, Wei L, Xiong Y, Yang A, Drobe B, Bao J, Zhou J, Gao Y, et al. Is peripheral motion detection affected by myopia?? Front NeuroSci. 2021;15:683153.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Kuo HY, Atchison DA, Schmid KL. Dot motion perception in young adult emmetropes and myopes. Optometry Vis Science: Official Publication Am Acad Optometry. 2018;95(6):498–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Vera-Diaz FA, Bex PJ, Ferreira A, Kosovicheva A. Binocular Temporal visual processing in myopia. J Vis. 2018;18(11):17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Gawecki M. Threshold Values of Myopic Anisometropia Causing Loss of Stereopsis. J Ophthalmol 2019, 2019:2654170.

  15. Khan N, Zaka-Ur-Rab S, Ashraf M, Mishra A. Comparison of stereoacuity in patients of anisometropia, isometropia and emmetropia. Indian J Ophthalmol. 2022;70(12):4405–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Ibironke JO, Friedman DS, Repka MX, Katz J, Giordano L, Hawse P, Tielsch JM. Child development and refractive errors in preschool children. Optometry Vis Science: Official Publication Am Acad Optometry. 2011;88(2):181–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Li X, Yang C, Zhang G, Zhang Y, Lan J, Chu H, Li J, Xie W, Wang S, Wiederhold BK, et al. Intermittent Exotropia treatment with dichoptic visual training using a unique virtual reality platform. Cyberpsychology Behav Social Netw. 2019;22(1):22–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Yang C, Li X, Zhang G, Lan J, Zhang Y, Chu H, Li J, Xie W, Wang S, Yan L, et al. Comparison of perceptual eye positions among patients with different degrees of anisometropia. Medicine. 2017;96(39):e8119.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Tan F, Yang X, Chu H, Yan L, Wiederhold BK, Wiederhold M, Liao Y. The study of perceptual eye position examination and visual perceptual training in postoperative intermittent exotropes. Cyberpsychology Behav Social Netw. 2020;23(12):871–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Zhao J, Luo W, Pang S, Hu B, Shui D, Bin L, Mao K, Hao G, Liu Z, Wiederhold BK, et al. Digital therapy for visual acuity and binocular function in children with anisometropic amblyopia. Cyberpsychology Behav Social Netw. 2023;26(12):924–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Lan J, Li Y, Pang S, Zhang G, Wu D, Yang C, Li J, Lin J, Yang X, Li Z, et al. Targeting displacement as an indicator of binocular fixation in normal Chinese adults. Front NeuroSci. 2023;17:1124034.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Nan L, LUWei SUNA-i, Hang YANL, Ling-ge C. The study of binocular rivalry of the children with amblyopia. YANKE. 2017;26(05):295–8.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Yang C, Huang W, Cui Y, Zhang G, Wang D, Xie W, Wiederhold M, Wiederhold B, Chu H, Yan L, et al. Binocular integration and stereopsis in children with television torticollis. BMC Ophthalmol. 2021;21(1):102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Hess RF, To L, Zhou J, Wang G, Cooperstock JR. Stereo vision: the Haves and Have-Nots. Iperception. 2015;6(3):2041669515593028.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Goseki T, Ishikawa H. The prevalence and types of strabismus, and average of stereopsis in Japanese adults. Jpn J Ophthalmol. 2017;61(3):280–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Liu L, Xu L, Guo J, Zhao L, Wu H. Evaluating the relationship between induced Aniseikonia and distance stereopsis. Ann Transl Med. 2023;11(2):37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Liu L, Xu L, Yu B, Zhao L, Wu H. The influence of simulated visual impairment on distance stereopsis. J Vis. 2024;24(2):1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Nishikawa N, Ishiko S, Yamaga I, Sato M, Yoshida A. Distance stereotesting using vision test charts for intermittent Exotropia. Clin Ophthalmol (Auckland NZ). 2015;9:1557–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Han SB, Yang HK, Kim J, Hong K, Lee B, Hwang JM. New stereoacuity test using a 3-dimensional display system in children. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(2):e0116626.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Wang X, Baldwin AS, Hess RF. Balanced binocular inputs support superior stereopsis. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2021;62(12):10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Morgan IG, Ohno-Matsui K, Saw SM. Myopia. Lancet. 2012;379(9827):1739–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Puthussery T, Venkataramani S, Gayet-Primo J, Smith RG, Taylor WR. NaV1.1 channels in axon initial segments of bipolar cells augment input to magnocellular visual pathways in the primate retina. J Neuroscience: Official J Soc Neurosci. 2013;33(41):16045–59.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Hubel DH, Livingstone MS. Segregation of form, color, and stereopsis in primate area 18. J Neuroscience: Official J Soc Neurosci. 1987;7(11):3378–415.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Gong JF, Xie HL, Mao XJ, Zhu XB, Xie ZK, Yang HH, Gao Y, Jin XF, Pan Y, Zhou F. Relevant factors of Estrogen changes of myopia in adolescent females. Chin Med J (Engl). 2015;128(5):659–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Mo Y, Wang MF, Zhou LL. Risk factor analysis of 167 patients with high myopia. Int J Ophthalmol. 2010;3(1):80–2.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Bar Dayan Y, Levin A, Morad Y, Grotto I, Ben-David R, Goldberg A, Onn E, Avni I, Levi Y, Benyamini OG. The changing prevalence of myopia in young adults: a 13-year series of population-based prevalence surveys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005;46(8):2760–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Wright LA, Wormald RP. Stereopsis and ageing. Eye (Lond). 1992;6(Pt 5):473–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Lu ZL, Dosher BA. Current directions in visual perceptual learning. Nat Rev Psychol. 2022;1(11):654–68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Dosher B, Lu ZL. Visual perceptual learning and models. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2017;3:343–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Lu ZL, Dosher BA. Perceptual learning retunes the perceptual template in foveal orientation identification. J Vis. 2004;4(1):44–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Wakayama A, Matsumoto C, Ohmure K, Inase M, Shimomura Y. Influence of background complexity on visual sensitivity and binocular summation using patterns with and without noise. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53(1):387–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Xu L, Huang M, Lan J, Huang W, Wang X, Zhang G, Li X, Shasha P, Chu H, Wiederhold BK, et al. Assessment of binocular imbalance with an augmented virtual reality platform in a normal population. Cyberpsychology Behav Social Netw. 2019;22(2):127–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Mao Y, Min SH, Chen S, Gong L, Chen H, Hess RF, Zhou J. Binocular imbalance in amblyopia depends on Spatial frequency in binocular combination. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2020;61(8):7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Pepin SM. Neuroadaptation of presbyopia-correcting intraocular lenses. Curr Opin Ophthalmol. 2008;19(1):10–2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Hafed ZM, Chen CY, Tian X. Vision, perception, and attention through the Lens of microsaccades: mechanisms and implications. Front Syst Neurosci. 2015;9:167.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Otero-Millan J, Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S. Fixational eye movements and binocular vision. Front Integr Nuerosci. 2014;8:52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Intoy J, Rucci M. Finely tuned eye movements enhance visual acuity. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):795.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Smith D, Ropar D, Allen HA. Does stereopsis account for the link between motor and social skills in adults? Mol Autism. 2018;9:55.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Levi DM, Knill DC, Bavelier D. Stereopsis and amblyopia: A mini-review. Vision Res. 2015;114:17–30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Livingstone MS, Lafer-Sousa R, Conway BR. Stereopsis and Artistic talent: poor stereopsis among Art students and established Artists. Psychol Sci. 2011;22(3):336–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Hong J, Kuo D, Su H, Li L, Guo Y, Chu H, Fu J. Ocular and visual perceptive factors associated with treatment outcomes in patients with anisometropic amblyopia. BMC Ophthalmol. 2023;23(1):21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Jiménez JR, Olivares JL, Pérez-Ocón F, del Barco LJ. Associated phoria in relation to stereopsis with random-dot stereograms. Optometry Vis Science: Official Publication Am Acad Optometry. 2000;77(1):47–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Guo Y, Fu J, Hong J, Liu Z, He X. Functional changes in the visual cortex in preoperative and postoperative patients with intermittent exotropia: study protocol for a non-randomised case–control clinical trial. BMJ Open. 2022;12(2):e055848.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This study was supported by the Guangxi Key Research and Development Program (No. Guike AB23026047), Guangxi Clinical Ophthalmic Research Center (No. Guike AD19245193), Guangxi Medical and Health Appropriate Technology Research and Development Project (S201550), and Guangdong Basic and Applied Basic Research Foundation (No. 2021A1515011822). All funding parties did not have any role in the study design, conduct of this study, data analysis, or decisions in preparation or publishing of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, XX and WQL; data analysis, XX; writing—original draft preparation, XX; data curation, XYY, LLL, EWL, MK, QC, JZ, LY; Investigation, YL, MK, QC; validation, EWL, MK, QC; writing-- reviewing and editing, YL, JZ, LY, WQL.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wuqiang Luo.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study adhered to the ethical principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki and received approval from the Ethics Committee of the People’s Hospital of Guangxi (KY-KJT-2023-285). All participating patients provided informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Xiao, X., Yang, X., Luo, Y. et al. Increasing myopic refraction reduces random dot stereopsis in Chinese myopic patients: a cross-sectional study. BMC Ophthalmol 25, 260 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12886-025-04098-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12886-025-04098-5

Keywords